Skip to content Skip to sidebar Skip to footer

(Download) "Culture and Gender Differences in Boredom Proneness (Report)" by North American Journal of Psychology * Book PDF Kindle ePub Free

Culture and Gender Differences in Boredom Proneness (Report)

📘 Read Now     📥 Download


eBook details

  • Title: Culture and Gender Differences in Boredom Proneness (Report)
  • Author : North American Journal of Psychology
  • Release Date : January 01, 2011
  • Genre: Education,Books,Professional & Technical,
  • Pages : * pages
  • Size : 193 KB

Description

Many scholars have suggested that boredom is a contributing factor to an assortment of personal, social, and organizational problems (Bernstein, 1975; Healy, 1984). Early work depicted boredom as a widespread "disease" (e.g., Mark, 1972; Ramey, 1974), an obstacle to mental health (Bernstein, 1975), and a profound personal and social issue (e.g., Healy, 1984; Iso-Ahola & Weissinger, 1987). Empirical data has generally supported the wide ranging and potentially harmful role of boredom. For instance, boredom has consistently been shown to be significantly related to negative affect such as depression, anxiety, and anger (Ahmed, 1990; Gana & Akremi, 1998; Gordon, Wilkinson, McGown, & Jovanoska, 1997; Rupp & Vodanovich, 1997; Sommers and Vodanovich, 2000; Vodanovich, Verner, & Gilbride, 1991). It has also been associated with detrimental behaviors such as pathological gambling, impulsivity, and procrastination (e.g., Blaszczynski, McConaghy, & Frankova, 1990; Blunt & Pychyl, 1998; Leong & Schneller, 1993; Vodanovich & Rupp, 1999; Watt & Vodanovich, 1992a), and with poor task/work performance (Kass, Vodanovich, & Callander; 2001; Kass, Vodanovich, Stanny, & Taylor; 2001; Sawin and Scerbo; 1995; Watt & Hargis, 2009). Despite the fact that a consensus does not exist regarding the definition of boredom, it has commonly been associated with several distinguishing features such as non-optimal (e.g., low) arousal levels, dissatisfaction, poor attentional control, and a monotonous, sparse environment (e.g., DeChenne & Moody, 1988; Farmer & Sundberg, 1986; Fisher, 1993; Hill & Perkins, 1985; Leary, Rogers, Canfield, & Coe, 1986; Mikulas & Vodanovich, 1993; Vodanovich & Kass, 1990). Further, distinctions exist in how boredom is defined in other languages. For instance, in German, the word for boredom is "langeweile" which essentially refers to the slow passing of time. In Hebrew, "leshaamem" means to long for and sadness due to being idle. A somewhat similar connotation exists with the French word for boredom, ennui, which generally refers to despondency and annoyance resulting from inactivity. On the other hand, the original meaning of the Slavic word depicting boredom, "kuka," is repetition and monotony (Waugh, 1975). Finally, Sundberg and Staat (1992) have reported that the Japanese, Chinese (Mandarin), and Korean words for boredom (or boring) emphasize the meanings of "nothing to do" and "not interesting" (see Esman, 1979; Peters, 1975; Waugh, 1975 for more information on the origin of the word boredom).


Ebook Download "Culture and Gender Differences in Boredom Proneness (Report)" PDF ePub Kindle